Biological determinism of human gender roles
This article duplicates the scope of other articles. (December 2017) |
This article contains weasel words: vague phrasing that often accompanies biased or unverifiable information. (December 2017) |
Biological determinism of human gender roles is the view that human sexuality is controlled by an individual's genes or some component of their physiology. This is an aspect of biological determinism, the wider view that human behaviour is determined by biological factors. Both views regard the contribution of learning and other environmental factors in a person's development as being over-stated.[1][2]
Of human gender roles[edit]
Gender assignment[edit]
Lynda Birke's In Pursuit of Difference argues that the discipline of human biology often presents "clear-cut differences"[3] between sexes with regards to chromosomes, genetics, and inheritance. However, while obvious physical differences between males and females exist and develop during puberty, hormonal differences are "not absolute".[4] There is a broad range of reproductive anatomy that doesn't necessarily fit the "gender definition" of male or female. According to the Intersex Society of North America, "a person may be born with mosaic genetics", differing in their chromosomal configuration.[5]
Homosexuality[edit]
Though scientists are unsure as to whether homosexuality can be attributed to biological or social factors, LGBT rights activists have used the theories of biological determinism to support their cause. This has become a frequent point of dissension between pro-gay individuals and anti-gay individuals. Due to the fact that a single cause has not been determined as the cause of homosexuality, many scholars theorize that a combination of biological and social causes determine one's sexual orientation.[6] Gay rights advocates believe that proving that homosexuality has a definite biological basis will prove it to be an unchangeable characteristic, thus allowing homosexuals to be protected under the Fourteenth Amendment in the United States.[7] One area of research that has been a valuable tool for gay rights activists has been Dean Hamer's work studying the "gay gene". Another researcher who worked with Hamer in finding evidence for biological influence in male homosexuality was Simon LeVay, a neuroscientist. In 1991, LeVay published an article in Science that detailed the difference in hypothalamic structures between homosexual and heterosexual men. His findings in studying the INAH-3 implied that "sexual orientation has a biological substrate".[8] Though his research showed that there was a biological basis in sexual orientation, LeVay cautioned against people interpreting his article to say that he found that homosexuality is genetic, emphasizing that he did not "locate a gay center in the brain—[as] INAH3 is less likely to be the sole gay nucleus of the brain than part of a chain of nuclei engaged in men and women's sexual behavior".[9] He merely hoped that his work would serve as a catalyst in working towards finding more evidence that homosexuality is at least partly genetic.
Social construction[edit]
This section has been nominated to be checked for its neutrality. (April 2017) (Learn how and when to remove this template message) |
Richard Lewontin, Steven Rose, and Leon Kamin were interested in the way that biological determinism was present in science. They wanted to figure out how much of it was true, and how much of it was socially constructed according to certain beliefs and societal norms and determined gender roles within society. In their book Not in Our Genes they explore the possibilities of biological determinism. In their studies, they found some very interesting evidence that points to the fact that biological determinism in science is actually greatly affected by certain norms and tendencies within society. According to them, biological determinism is more constructed by society than by anything else. In a study that was performed on girls who were relatively "masculinized", biological determinists John Money and Anke Ehrhardt looked for ways to describe femininity that fit into the common definition of it, such as clothing preference or using makeup. Although these scientists believed that they were providing evidence to support their definitions of femininity within nature, they fell into the trap of labeling these girls according to Western social standards. As Lewontin points out, this experiment not only embraces the stereotypes that already existed, but it also "ignores the existence of societies in which women wear pants, or in which men wear skirts, or in which men enjoy and appropriate jewelry to themselves."[10] Lewontin, Rose, and Kamin realize that biological determinism is clouded and, can in fact, be shaped according to the standards and norms of the society one lives in. Therefore, they choose to take a different approach. They decide to look at numbers and statistics instead of simple social experiments which can be easily misinterpreted. When they look at the numbers and statistics of men and women over the years, they discover that the differences between men and women are no longer as pronounced as they had been in the past. All of a sudden, there are more women in the work place holding higher ranking jobs. More women are excelling in areas that used to be male dominant, such as sports. And, even biologically, women are beginning to catch up to men in height while men are beginning to catch up in life expectancy. However, these changes are mostly visible in numbers and statistics, and social differences between men and women are still easily observed. Lewontin, Rose, and Kamin argue, however, that these differences are imposed by society itself.
The standard model for the difference between sex/gender states that there is a clear-cut dichotomy between males & females, with no overlap. Margaret W. Rossiter’s article “Women’s Work in Science” discusses how in the 1880s women were introduced to the science field and hired, but although employed they were still segregated from men in the science field.[11] According to Rossiter, segregation started because it was apparent that men and women worked differently and had different "talents" to offer in the field of science.[11] To add to this, women in the 1880s got paid less than the male worker in the science field. Women got paid an average of "two hundred dollars less" than their male coworkers yearly.[12] Because of this norm there is a historically constructed viewpoint of "average", meaning that society holds the idea that one must be either male or female, feminine or masculine. Anne Fausto-Sterling's article "Of Gender and Genitals" discusses how this standard model shapes doctors' ideas about gender and what is socially acceptable.[13] She claims that (according to the standard model) "Bodies in the "normal" range are culturally intelligible as males or females, but the rules for living as male or female are strict", meaning that people are culturally "trained" in believing that there is a sexual binary and anything outside of those confines is rejected.
References[edit]
- ↑ Taylor A., Verta; Rupp, Leila J.; Whittier, Nancy, eds. (2012). Feminist frontiers (9th ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill. ISBN 9780078026621. Search this book on
- ↑ Naiman, Joanne (2012). How societies work: class, power, and change (5th ed.). Halifax, Winnipeg: Fernwood Publishing. ISBN 9781552664810. Search this book on
- ↑ Birke, Lynda (2000). "In pursuit of difference: scientific studies of women and men". Feminism and the biological body. New Brunswick, New Jersey: Rutgers University Press. ISBN 9780813528236. Search this book on
- ↑ Laurie, Timothy (3 June 2015), "Bigotry or biology: the hard choice for an opponent of marriage equality", The Drum, Australia: ABC, retrieved 9 April 2017
- ↑ Staff writer. "Home page". isna.org. Intersex Society of North America. Retrieved 9 April 2017.
- ↑ Abramson, Paul R.; Pinkerton, Steven D. (1995). "Introduction: Nature, nurture, and in-between". Sexual nature, sexual culture. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. p. 4. ISBN 9780226001821. Search this book on
- ↑ Brookey, Robert Alan (2001). "Bio-rhetoric, background beliefs and the biology of homosexuality". Argumentation and Advocacy. Taylor and Francis. 37 (4): 171–183. doi:10.1080/00028533.2001.11951668.
- ↑ Spanier, Bonnie (Spring 1995). "Biological determinism and homosexuality". NWSA Journal. Johns Hopkins University Press. 7 (1): 54–71. JSTOR 4316378.
- ↑ Nimmons, David (March 1994). "Sex and the brain". Discover Magazine. Retrieved 9 April 2017.
- ↑ Lewontin, Richard C.; Rose, Steven; Kamin, Leon J. (1984). "The determined patriarchy" (PDF). Not in our genes: biology, ideology, and human nature. New York: Pantheon Books. pp. 132–163. ISBN 9780140226058. Search this book on
- ↑ 11.0 11.1 Rossiter, Margaret W. (September 1980). ""Women's Work" in Science, 1880-1910". Isis. University of Chicago Press. 71 (3): 381–398. JSTOR 230118.
- ↑ Jardins, Julie (2010). "To embrace or decline marriage and family: Annie Jump Cannon and the women of the Harvard Observatory, 1880-1940". The Madame Curie complex: the hidden history of women in science. New York: Feminist Press at the City University of New York. p. 92. ISBN 9781558616134. Search this book on
- ↑ Fausto-Sterling, Anne (2000). "Of gender and genitals: the use and abuse of the modern intersexual" (PDF). Sexing the body. New York: Basic Books. pp. 44–77. ISBN 9780465077144. Search this book on
This article "Biological determinism of human gender roles" is from Wikipedia. The list of its authors can be seen in its historical and/or the page Edithistory:Biological determinism of human gender roles. Articles copied from Draft Namespace on Wikipedia could be seen on the Draft Namespace of Wikipedia and not main one.